Impaired cortical tracking of speech in children with developmental language disorder
Nora, Anni; Rinkinen, Oona; Renvall, Hanna; Service, Elisabet; Arkkila, Eva; Smolander, Sini; Laasonen, Marja; Salmelin, Riitta (2024-04-08)
Nora, Anni
Rinkinen, Oona
Renvall, Hanna
Service, Elisabet
Arkkila, Eva
Smolander, Sini
Laasonen, Marja
Salmelin, Riitta
Society of Neuroscience
08.04.2024
Nora, A., Rinkinen, O., Renvall, H., Service, E., Arkkila, E., Smolander, S., Laasonen, M., & Salmelin, R. (2024). Impaired cortical tracking of speech in children with developmental language disorder. The Journal of Neuroscience, 44(22), e2048232024. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2048-23.2024
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
© 2024 Nora et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
© 2024 Nora et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
Julkaisun pysyvä osoite on
https://urn.fi/URN:NBN:fi:oulu-202405283997
https://urn.fi/URN:NBN:fi:oulu-202405283997
Tiivistelmä
Abstract
In developmental language disorder (DLD), learning to comprehend and express oneself with spoken language is impaired, but the reason for this remains unknown. Using millisecond scale magnetoencephalography (MEG) recordings combined with machine learning models, we investigated whether the possible neural basis of this disruption lies in poor cortical tracking of speech. The stimuli were common spoken words (e.g., ‘dog’, ‘car’, ‘hammer’) and sounds with corresponding meanings (e.g., dog bark, car engine, hammering). In both children with DLD (10 boys and 7 girls) and typically developing (TD) control children (14 boys and 3 girls), aged 10-15 years, the cortical activation to spoken words was best modeled as time-locked to the unfolding speech input at ∼100 ms latency between sound and cortical activation. Amplitude envelope (amplitude changes) and spectrogram (detailed time-varying spectral content) of the spoken words, but not other sounds, were very successfully decoded based on time-locked brain responses in bilateral temporal areas; based on the cortical responses, the models could tell at ∼75-85% accuracy, which of two sounds had been presented to the participant. However, the cortical representation of the amplitude envelope information was poorer in children with DLD compared to TD children at longer latencies (at ∼200–300 ms lag). We interpret this effect as reflecting poorer retention of acoustic-phonetic information in short-term memory. This impaired tracking could potentially affect the processing and learning of words as well as continuous speech. The present results offer an explanation for the problems in language comprehension and acquisition in DLD.
In developmental language disorder (DLD), learning to comprehend and express oneself with spoken language is impaired, but the reason for this remains unknown. Using millisecond scale magnetoencephalography (MEG) recordings combined with machine learning models, we investigated whether the possible neural basis of this disruption lies in poor cortical tracking of speech. The stimuli were common spoken words (e.g., ‘dog’, ‘car’, ‘hammer’) and sounds with corresponding meanings (e.g., dog bark, car engine, hammering). In both children with DLD (10 boys and 7 girls) and typically developing (TD) control children (14 boys and 3 girls), aged 10-15 years, the cortical activation to spoken words was best modeled as time-locked to the unfolding speech input at ∼100 ms latency between sound and cortical activation. Amplitude envelope (amplitude changes) and spectrogram (detailed time-varying spectral content) of the spoken words, but not other sounds, were very successfully decoded based on time-locked brain responses in bilateral temporal areas; based on the cortical responses, the models could tell at ∼75-85% accuracy, which of two sounds had been presented to the participant. However, the cortical representation of the amplitude envelope information was poorer in children with DLD compared to TD children at longer latencies (at ∼200–300 ms lag). We interpret this effect as reflecting poorer retention of acoustic-phonetic information in short-term memory. This impaired tracking could potentially affect the processing and learning of words as well as continuous speech. The present results offer an explanation for the problems in language comprehension and acquisition in DLD.
Kokoelmat
- Avoin saatavuus [34516]